5001309

download 5001309

of 6

Transcript of 5001309

  • 7/25/2019 5001309

    1/6

    Is Impulse Purchasing Really a Useful

    on ept for Marketing Decisions?

    DA VID T KOLLAT

    a n d

    RON ALD P WILLETT

    Impulse purchasing is an

    important aspect o f custom-

    er in-store behavior as dis-

    cussed in the previous ar-

    ticle. The authors of this

    paper ind icate that the con-

    ept as presently employ ed

    as limited usefulness as a

    dec i -

    66 TM PU LS E or unplanned purchasing is a familiar term t

    I

    most business executives and mark eting academicians.^ In

    deed, it is common to refer to certa in prod ucts as imp ulse item s

    and or to account for certain kinds of behavio r by classifyin

    it as impulse purchasing.

    Unplanned purchasing is, of course, not confined to any produc

    or reta il settin g. The phenomenon has been used to describe pur

    chases of such products as durable goods,- jewelry, wearing ap

    parel, hardware items, furniture,^ drugs and toiletries^ and grocer

    pro du cts. ' Moreover, the behavior has been found to occur i

    such retail settings as drugstores, superm arkets, ' departmen

    store s, and variety and specialty stores inclu ding gift shops, flor

    ists,

    book, barb er, hardw are, auto supply, lumber and furn itur

    stores.**

    While the above studies indicate that unplanned purchasing oc

    curs in many types of retail outlets, it is most commonly studie

    in super ma rkets. The importance of this type of behavior in thi

    retail settin g is documented by the latest du Pont study, whic

    indicates that unplanned purchasing accounts for 50rc of the prod

    ucts purchased in food supermarkets.

    Several kinds of retailers, particularly supermarket executive

    and managers, allegedly use unplanned purchasing as a criterio

    for decisions about merchandise location, space allocation and dif

    1 As used in this paper, impulse and unplan ned are synonymous.

    -

    George Katona and Eva Mueller, A Study of Purcha se D ecisions

    in Lincoln Clark, editor.

    The Dynamics of Consumer Reactions

    (New

    Yo rk: New York U niversity Pres s. 1955), pp. 30-88.

    Vernon T. Clover, Relative Im portanc e of Impulse Buy ing in Reta

    S to re s, JOURNAL OF MARKETING, Vol. 25 (J u ly , 1 95 0) , pp . 66-70.

    * Drugstore Brand Sw itching and Impulse Buying

    (New York: Poi

    of Purchase Advertising Insti tute, 1963).

    Consumer Buying Habits Studies

    (E . I. du Pont de Nem ours and C

    1945, 1949, 1954, 1959, 19 65 ); C. Joh n W est. Re sult s of Two Y ea

    of Study into Impulse Buying. JOURNAL OF MARKETING, V'ol.

    (Ja nu ary . 1951). pp. 362-363; Haw kins Stern, The Significance o

    Impulse Buying Today,

    JOURNAL OF MARKETING.

    Vol. 26 (Apr

    1962),

    pp. 59-62; and Jam es D. Schaffer, Th e Influence of Impul

    Buying or In-the-Store Decisions on Consumer Food Purchase

    (Jou rnal pap er no. 2591 from the Michigan Agric ultu ral Exp er

    mental Stat ion) .

    Same reference as footnote 4, p. 19.

    Consumer Buying Habits Studies

    (E . I. du Pont de Nem ours and C

    1965),

    pp. 3-4.

    Same reference as footnote 3, p. 67.

  • 7/25/2019 5001309

    2/6

    80

    Journal of Marketing, January, 196

    feren tial promotion trea tm ent of items. Certain

    store layouts, product locations, shelf locations, and

    types of displays are apparently thought to be more

    conducive to and consistent with unplanned pur-

    chasing than are others. Unplanned purch asing is

    also of interest to m anu factu rers. Packaging and

    point-of-purchase decisions allegedly are based, in

    part, on a product's present or potential rate of

    unplanned purchasing.'"

    Thus,

    the importance and relevance of unplanned

    purchasing is apparently widely recognized and ac-

    cepted. However, is the concept really a useful

    basis for the types of marketing decisions that have

    been mentioned above? Th is article discusses four

    basic problems with unplanned purchasing that

    severely limit its usefulness for marketing decisions.

    First, the concept is too vague and encompasses too

    many types of behavior to be an operational criterion

    for marketing decisions. Second measured un-

    planned purchasing rates may be exaggerated be-

    cause of the methodology used to generate them,

    and may distort the potential for increasing prod-

    ucts' sales by manipulation of in-store location and

    display. Third present measures of unplanned pur-

    chasing rates are misleading bases for selecting

    products for differential promotional treatments.

    Fourth, unplanned purchase rates for individual

    brands may differ significantly from the parent

    product category rate, thereby deceiving both re-

    tailers and manufacturers .

    The Problems of Definition

    Since unplanned purchasing is a widely recognized

    and frequently talked about type of behavior, it

    would seem logical to expect some consensus about

    the mean ing of the phenomenon. Actually, this is

    not the case, for the term is used in many different

    ways. The following are illustrative of the variety

    of conceptual definitions:

    (1) An impulse purchase is an unplanned, spur

    of the moment decision to purchase a prod-

    2) An impulse purchase is a logical and effi-

    cient way of making purchase decisions, since

    by waiting until one is in the store to final-

    ize purchase intentions, a more comprehen-

    sive and realistic evaluation of purchase

    alternatives can often be made.i-

    1" Bert C. McCammon, Jr ., "The Role of Point-of-P ur-

    chase Display in the Manufacturer's Marketing Mix,"

    in Taylor Meloan and Charles Whitlo (editors).

    Com-

    petition in Marketing (Los Angeles: Graduate School

    of Business, University of Southern California, 1964),

    p.

    78.

    William R. Davidson and Alton F. Doody,R etailing

    (3) There is no such thing as an impulse pu

    chase. Rather, the re are four types of u

    planned purchases: (1) Pure impulse is

    novelty or escape purchase which breaks

    normal buying pattern,

    2) Reminder impul

    occurs when a shopper sees an item or r

    calls an advertisement or other informatio

    and remembers that the stock at home is lo

    or exhausted, (3) Suggestion impulse pu

    chasing occurs when a shopper sees a pro

    uct for the first time and visualizes a ne

    for it, and (4) Planned impulse purchasi

    takes place when the shopper makes specif

    purchase decisions on the basis of price sp

    cials, coupon offers, and the like.'^

    Field studies of unplanned purchasing have e

    ployed still other operation al definitions. Th e f

    lowing are illustrative:

    (4) Shoppers are questioned upon entering t

    store as to what they plan to purchase a

    records are made of what they do in fact pu

    chase. Those items purchased but not me

    tioned during the first interview are impu

    purchases.

    (5) Impulse purchasing is the difference in p

    chases between a sample of customers

    porting actual purchases (exposed to in-sto

    stimuli) and another sample of customers

    porting what they anticipated buying wh

    Hawkins Stern, "The Significance of Impulse Buyi

    Today," JOURNAL OF MARKETING. Vol. 26 (Ap

    1962), p. 59.

    Same reference as footnote 7, p. 2.

    ABOUT THE AUTHORS. Dav id T.

    KoUort is an Associate Prolessor of Mar-

    keting at The Ohio State University. Di.

    Kollat has published articles in theJour-

    nal ot Marketinq Research the Proceed-

    ings of the American Marketing Associ-

    ation,

    and has been a contributor to the

    Ejicyclopaedia Britannica. He is coau-

    thor of Consumer Behavior and Cases

    in Consumer Behavior

    and is als o a co-

    author of a forthcoming book Research

    in Consumer Behavior.

    Ronald P. Willett is Associate Profes-

    sor of Marketing and Assistant Chair-

    man. Marketing Department, Indiana

    University. He holds BS and MBA de-

    gr ee s from Miami Univ ersity Oxford)

    and his DBA is from Indiana University.

    Dr.

    Willett is currently Director of the

    Pharmacists Buying Behavior Study and

  • 7/25/2019 5001309

    3/6

    Is Impulse Purchasing Really a Useful Concept for Marketing Decisions?

    sitting in their living rooms (not exposed to

    in-store stimuli) .'^

    (6) Impulse purchasing is the difference in a

    store's sales volume during weeks in which

    a holiday occurred with the week immediately

    following during which a holiday did not

    occur.'^^

    From the above, it appears that there are con-

    siderable differences of opinion as to what is meant

    by an impulse pur cha se.' Conceptual definitions

    diflfer not only in degrees of precision but, more

    basically, in terms of the amount and type of deci-

    sion making that is involved, and, by implication,

    the susceptibility of the behavior to marketer strat-

    egies. Sim ilarly, there is considerable var iation in

    the way in which impulse purchasing is empirically

    efined. Fina lly, and perha ps most imp ortan tly,

    there are conspicuous differences between conceptual

    and empirical definitions.

    The lack of consensus about the meaning of im-

    pulse purchasing would seem to limit the usefulness

    f the concept. The different definitions used in

    empirical studies make it difficult to compare find-

    ings and accumulate information about the nature

    of the behavior. Moreover, the mark eting implica-

    tions of impulse buying sometimes vary according

    to which definition is adopted. Fo r example, if

    definition (1) is accepted, it may be advantag eous

    o periodically relocate products having high un-

    lanned purchasing rates in order to increase cus-

    omer expo sure. In co ntr ast , if definition

    (2)

    is

    ularly if the location(s) is inconsistent with where

    Finally, and perhaps most basic, it is questionable

    or what it involves. Th at is, how

    Impulse Pureh as ing Fact or A rt i fac t?

    There are differences of opinion about why im-

    se purc hasin g occurs. Two conflicting explana-

    Jam es D. Schaffer, The Influence of Impulse Buying

    or In-the-Store Decisions on Consumer Food Pur-

    chases (Jou rna l paper no. 2591 from the Michigan

    Agricultural Experimental Station).

    Same reference as footnote 3. p. 68.

    For other definitions of impulse purchasing, seeHow

    opl

    Shop for Food

    (Market Research Corporation

    of America, undated manuscript) ; One On The Aisle

    The Exposure Hypothesis

    According to this hypothesis, exposure to in-store

    stimuli produces unplanned purchases because: (1)

    shoppers use product assortments and other in-store

    stimuli to remind them of their shopping needs;

    that is, shoppers make some purchase decisions in

    the store rather than relying solely on a shopping

    list; and/or (2) in-store promotional techniques

    result in shoppers recognizing new ways of satis-

    fying needs.

    Several studies have indicated that in-store stim-

    uli can trig ge r unplanned purchases. For example,

    it has been demonstrated that the store location of

    such products as bread, milk and dairy products is

    instrumental in precipitating unplanned purchases

    of surrou ndin g products. * Point-of-purchase dis

    plays, signs.^'-' and shelf extenders'- are illustrativ

    of other promotional techniques that have, in some

    instances, proven effective in stimulating impulse

    purchases.

    However, increasing product exposure does not

    always increase the rate of unplanned purchasing

    For example, one study has shown that some prod-

    ucts with relatively high impulse purchasing rates

    (Tang, hominy, powdered coffee cream) are not more

    responsive to increases in shelf spacethan the sales

    of products having relatively low impulse purchasing

    rates (bakin g so da ).- ' Moreover, in some instances,

    end-aisle displays of relatively high impulse prod

    ucts (pie crust mix and apple pie filling) do no

    produce greater direct product profits than the end-

    aisle treatment of relatively low impulse products

    (spag hetti and spa ghe tti sauce) .-'-

    Thus, contrary to what seems to be the opinion

    of many marketing academicians and practitioners

    exposure to in-store stimuli triggers only some un

    planned purchasing, not all of it. The rat e of un-

    planned purchasing appears to depend on the type

    of stimulation technique, the product that is being

    1* Lawrence W. Patterson, In-Store Traffic Flow (Ne

    York: Point-of-Purchase Advertising, Institute, Inc.

    1963),

    p. 2.

    '^ See, for example. ^4

    First Study of Totality o f Impac

    of Point-of-Purchase Material on Plus Sales in Super

    markets

    ( N e w

    Y or k : Po in t - o f - Pur c ha se A dve r t i s i n

    I ns t i t u t e , I nc . ,1959) , p . 18 ;

    Drugstore Brand Siritch

    ing an d Impulse Buying ( N e w Y or k : Po in t - o f - Pu

    chase Adver t i s ing Ins t i tute , I nc . ,1963) , p . 10 ;

    Pac

    age Store Brand Sw-itching an d Impulse Buying

    ( N e w

    Y or k : Po in t - o f - Pur c ha se A dve r t i s i ng I ns t i t u t e , 1963)

    p.

    10.

    -* Sa mu el Ro uda ,

    W hat Today's Supermarket Chain

    Expect from Manufacturers

    in

    Promotional Suppo

    ( N e w Y or k : Po in t - o f - Pur c ha se A dve r t i s i ng I ns t i t u t e

    1 9 5 7 ) , p . 64 .

    21 K eith

    C o x , T h e

    Re spons ive ne ss

    of

    Food Sales

    t

    Shelf Space Changes in S u p e r m a r k e t s ,

    Joumal o

    Marketing Research, V o l . 1 ( M ay ,1964) , p p . 63-6

  • 7/25/2019 5001309

    4/6

    8

    Journal of Marketing January 19

    promoted, and the customer who selectively exposes

    himself to, and selectively perceives, the promotional

    stim uli. If in-store stimuli do not precip itate all

    unplanned purchasing, then it follows that other

    factors must be causing some of this type of

    behavior.

    The Custonter-commitment Hypothesis

    The customer-commitment hypothesis maintains

    unplanned purchasing, or differences between pur-

    chase intentions and actual purchases, are attribut-

    able, in part, to incomplete measures of purchase

    plaris.

    Measured purchase intention s are not the

    same as actual purchase inten tion s because the

    shopper is iinicilling and/or unable to commit the

    time and/or cognitive resources necessary to make

    the two types of purchase intentions equivalent.

    The customer may be

    ununlling

    to itemize her

    purchase intentions because she does not want to

    invest the amount of time and thought necessary

    to give the interviewer a complete roster of her

    purchase plans. Instead, she artic ula tes only an

    incomplete itemization of what she plans to purchase,

    thereby satisfying the requirements of the interview

    without spending too much time or having to think

    too much.

    There are several reasons why the shopper may be

    unable to itemize her purch ase plans. Fir st, the

    shopper may know what she will purchase but may

    be unable to articulate these purchase intentions

    because of the characteristics of the interview.

    The methodology used in most .studies of unplanned

    purchasing forces the shopper, in the absence of a

    shopping list, to rely on memory for purchase in-

    tentio ns. In other words, unaided and nearly spon-

    taneous recall is usually used to measure purchase

    plans. Thus the methodology alone guarantees that

    measured purchase plans will deviate to some degree

    from actual purchase plans.

    Second, the shopper may know what she will pur-

    chase but may be unable to relate these intentions

    regardless of the amount of assistance given by the

    interview er. Tha t is. with out exposure to in-store

    stimuli, the shopper may be unable to construct

    cognitively and verbalize for the interviewer what

    she will purchase.

    The logic underlying the customer-commitment

    hypothesis was alluded to nearly two decades ago.-'-^

    The hypothesis was also deduced from a recent study

    of the unplanned purchasing behavior of 600 super-

    m ark et shoppers.-' Since the hyp othe sis was a by-

    product rather than an objective of the study, it

    was not pos.sible to determine the amount of un-

    3

    William A pplebaum, Stud ying Customer Behavior

    in Retail Stores, JOURNAL OF MARKETING. Vol. 16

    planned purchasing caused by the phenomena co

    prising the hypothesis.

    Implications

    To the extent that unplanned purchasing is

    tributable to the customer-commitment hypothes

    unplanned purchasing is not unplanned at all, b

    rather an artifact of the way in which the beh

    ior is mea sured. Conseq uently, true unplanned p

    chase rates are considerably lower than these th

    are currently accepted. Tha t is to say, pre-shoppi

    decisions about products and brands to be p

    chased are more common than past studies ha

    indicated.

    The evidence presented indicates that presen

    accepted rates of unplanned purchasing-' may

    aggerate the potential for influencing buying de

    sions.

    As a consequence, promotional exp enditu

    designed to increase the rate of unplanned purch

    ing may be excessive. Conclusions concerning

    degree of excessiveness must be postponed until

    quantitative impact of the customer-commitment h

    pothesis is determined.

    Promotional Implications

    Retailers sometimes use product unplanned p

    chasing rates to select products for differential p

    motional tre atm en ts. Such decisions as store lo

    tion, shelf height, number of shelf facings, and e

    aisle treatment for specific products are often bas

    in part, on product unplanned purchase rates.

    Since some unplanned purchasing is not unplann

    at all, but an artifact of the way in which the

    havior is measured, trve unplanned purchas

    rates are considerably lower than those that

    cur ren tly accepted. It seems risky to assume t

    all product unplanned purchasing rates are infla

    to the same degree; rather, some product rates

    probably overstated more than are others.

    Research to date does not facilitate measuring

    extent of distortion of unplanned rates across pr

    ucts but the direction of bias can be suggest

    Based on the customer commitment explanation

    bias in unplanned rates is likely to be high for ite

    that are discretionary rather than staple, or h

    low frequencies of purchase or have highly varia

    interpurchase time periods. These same charac

    istics are purported to be determinants of unplan

    purchase rate s. Hence, product categories with h

    measured unplanned purchase rates are also likely

    demonstrate greater measurement error, and c

    versely, products with low unplanned purchase r

    exhib it smaller am ounts of err or . As a conseque

    there may be a smaller range between prod

    purported to have high and low unplanned purch

    rates, and the use of these rates as a single crite

    for assignment of promotional effort may be q

  • 7/25/2019 5001309

    5/6

    Is Impulse Purchasing Realty a Useful Concept for Marketing Decisions?

    83

    Thus, although direct empirical evidence is not

    available, from the evidence presented above it ap-

    pears legitimate to deduce that existing unplanned

    purchase rates are likely to be misleading bases for

    selecting products for promotional efforts.

    Product Category Rates May Be Misleading

    Unplanned purchasing rates are typically reported

    by product category.-* It is often dangerous to as-

    sume that a specific brand's unplanned purchasing

    rate is equal to the product category average.

    In one instance studied, for example, a food prod-

    uct category had an average unplanned purchase

    rate of

    b\^c.

    Although most of the brands in this

    category had unplanned rates approximating

    bl ^ c,

    the unplanned rate for one major brand was only

    26 27

    For reasons already mentioned, the above figures

    may not represent the

    true

    unplanned rates. How-

    ever, for purposes of discussion, assume that they

    are correct. If so. then the product category rate

    may be misleading to both retailers and manu-

    facturers.

    If retailers fail to recognize that the unplanned

    rates of individu al bra nd s may differ significantly

    from the product category rate, then,

    all other things

    being equal,

    they are likely to be indifferent as to

    which brand within a product category is given spe-

    cial promotional treatm ent . In the above case this

    may be a mistake, since sales of the brand having

    the lower unplanned rate may be less responsive to

    ome special promotional efforts. Th us, basing pro-

    otion decisions on product category averages rather

    Product category rates may also be misleading to

    ufactu rers. In the above case, for example, the

    w rate . Does it mean tha t he is doing

    Why is he not gettin g a competitive sh are

    f in-store purch ase decisions? The point, of course,

    Toward an Operational Concept of

    Impulse Purchas ing

    Unplanned Purchasing Can Become a

    From the discussion above it is apparent that sev-

    eral problems must be overcome before unplanned

    purchasing can become a useful concept for market-

    ing decisions.

    First,

    the concept must be precisely

    defined. Since the value of the concept h inge s in

    large part on empirical studies of the extent and

    nature of the behavior, it seems desirable to adop

    a definition used in empirical studies, providing

    that it is capable of yielding managerially relevant

    insights.

    Second

    a series of methodological studies

    needs to be conducted in order to devise a measuring

    instrument and process that will equate measured

    and actual purchase intentions. Space limitations

    prohibit a detailed presentation of such studies here

    In general, however, it would appear useful to de-

    termine experimentally the effects of interviewer

    cueing and customer incentives on the rate of un-

    planned purchasing. Experimental manipulation of

    interviewer cueing would attempt to determine the

    extent to which inability to itemize purchase inten-

    tions artificially create s subsequent unplanned pur-

    chas ing. Variou s levels and types of incentives could

    be used to overcome customer unwillingness to artic-

    ulate purchase intentions.

    Finally,

    unplanned pur

    chasing ra tes need to be computed for individua

    brands as well as product categories.

    Overcoming these problems will involve a substan-

    tial commitment of time and resources. However, the

    potential value of

    true

    rates of unplanned purchas

    ing may exceed the costs of obtaining them. When

    true

    rates are determined, other categories of plan

    ningspecifically planned, generally planned, and

    brand substitutionwould probably differ, both in

    magnitude and in relative occurrence, from those

    that are currently accepted. These refined measures

    of various types of in-store decisions would provide

    more sensitive indices of the amount and type of

    promotional effort that should be allocated to

    products.

    Refined measures of unplanned purchasing would

    permit a partial functional analysis of the strength

    and weaknesses of a firm's promotional program

    For example, if unplanned purchasing were defined

    and measured in such a way as to be equivalent to

    in-store purchase decisions, a manufacturer could

    use rates of unplanned p urchasing for his brand (si

    as a criterion for evaluating the effectiveness of both

    his advertising and in-store promotional strategy

    Pure

    measures of unplanned purchasing and othe

    categories of planning would constitute one of th

    most potentially meaningful indices of the real effect

    of specific in-store product promotions. For example

    the difference in a product's unplanned purchase rat

    before and after a special in-store promotion could

    be used by the retailer as a measure of the effective

    ness of that strategy. Other planning categorie

    could also be used in this manner. These m easure

    of effectiveness seem particularly useful since the

  • 7/25/2019 5001309

    6/6