The Temporal Pattern of Shame and Guilt

download The Temporal Pattern of Shame and Guilt

of 29

Transcript of The Temporal Pattern of Shame and Guilt

  • 8/13/2019 The Temporal Pattern of Shame and Guilt

    1/29

    The Temporal Pattern of Shame and Guilt

    21 January 2014

    Joost Knaap

    S1715267

    Supervisor: Dr Kai Epstude

    Faculty of Behavioral and Social Sciences

    University of Groningen

  • 8/13/2019 The Temporal Pattern of Shame and Guilt

    2/29

    The temporal pattern of Shame and Guilt

    2

    Samenvatting

    In eerder onderzoek werd bewijs gevonden dat acties (iets gedaan hebben) meer spijt tot gevolg

    hebben op de korte termijn, terwijl passiviteit (iets niet gedaan hebben) juist meer spijt tot gevolg

    heeft naarmate een langere tijdsduur verstrijkt. In deze scriptie wordt op deze resultaten

    voortgebouwd. Onderzocht werd of een dergelijk tijd-effect ook bestaat voor de emoties

    schaamte en schuld. Schaamte is een emotie die zich vooral richt op het zelf, terwijl schuld zich

    richt op situaties. De hypothese in het onderzoek was dat voor de ervaring van schaamte niet zou

    uitmaken of sprake was van iets gedaan of niet gedaan hebben, in tegenstelling met de ervaring

    van schuld. Dit omdat het voor mensen moeilijk is voor te stellen datzijzelfanders zouden zijn

    geweest (zelfbeeld is stabiel door de tijd heen), maar daarentegen juist makkelijker om zich voor

    te stellen dat eensituatieanders zou zijn geweest. Deelnemers lazen verschillende versies vanscenarios die ofwel schaamte ofwel schuld opriepen. Daarnaast werd ook handelen versus niet

    handelen en tijdsduur gemanipuleerd, hetgeen resulteerde in een 2x2x2 tussen proefpersonen

    onderzoeksopzet. De hypothese werd niet bevestigd. Wel werden significante verschillen

    gevonden tussen gebeurtenissen die zich kort gelden (1 dag) en lang gelden (6 maanden) hadden

    voorgedaan voor het schuld-scenario. Daarnaast gaven proefpersonen die het schaamte-scenario

    hadden gelezen aan meer schaamte te ervaren als gevolg van iets dat ze gedaan hadden ten

    opzichte van iets dat ze niet gedaan hadden. De implicaties hiervan voor toekomstig onderzoekworden besproken.

  • 8/13/2019 The Temporal Pattern of Shame and Guilt

    3/29

    The temporal pattern of Shame and Guilt

    3

    Abstract

    In previous research it has been found that actions produce more regret in the short term, but

    contrarily- failures to act produce more regret in the long run. This thesis draws on these

    findings, investigating whether such a temporal effect also exists for the other counterfactual

    emotions shame and guilt. It is hypothesized that for guilt such an effect would exist, but not for

    shame. Participants read about situations evoking either shame or guilt, over actions and

    inactions (event type) on the short-term or long run (2x2x2 between subjects design). The results

    did not give support for this hypothesis. However, significant differences between events that

    happened in the short term (1 day ago) compared to events that happened in the long run (6

    months ago) were found for the participants that read a guilt-inducing scenario. When

    participants were presented with the shame scenario a significant difference between event type(action and inaction) was found: participants reported more shame because of actions compared

    to inactions. Implications for further research are discussed.

    Keywords: shame, guilt, regret, counterfactual thinking, simulation heuristic, norm theory,

    action, inaction

  • 8/13/2019 The Temporal Pattern of Shame and Guilt

    4/29

    The temporal pattern of Shame and Guilt

    4

    Foreword

    I would like to thank Kai for his supervision of this thesis. Not only did he give constructive

    feedback and inspiration, he also also places a high value in interpersonal relations. I remember

    him, when we had an appointment, always first asking how I was doing before going to

    business. I did appreciate that a lot. I would also like to thank my parents for their support

    throughout my studies and Amy for proofreading parts of it.

    Conducting a large research project and writing a thesis about is was not always an easy job.

    However, I found and still find that counterfactual emotions (the topic of this thesis) is one of the

    most interesting and compelling subjects in (social) psychology and I am glad to have been able

    to explore it and maybe even having contributed a little to the understanding of the complex

    functioning of these emotions. Writing this thesis has been a valuable experience.

    Amsterdam, January 2014

    Joost Knaap

  • 8/13/2019 The Temporal Pattern of Shame and Guilt

    5/29

    The temporal pattern of Shame and Guilt

    5

    For all sad words of tongue or pen, the saddest are these: It might have been!

    -- John Greenleaf Whittier (1989)

    If we didnt have birthdays, you wouldnt be you

    If youd never been born, well then what would you do?

    If youd never been born, well then what would you be?

    You mightbe a fish! Or a toad in a tree!

    You might be a doorknob! Or three baked potatoes!

    You might be a bag full of hard green tomatoes!

    Or worse than that.. You might be a WASNT!

    A Wasnt has no fun at all. No, he doesnt.

    A Wasnt just isnt. he isnt present.

    But you You are YOU! And, now isnt that pleasant.

    -- Theodor Seuss Geisel (1959)

    Every path is the right path. Everything could have been anything else and it would have just as

    much meaning.

    -- Quote from the movieMr Nobody(Van Dormael, 2009)

  • 8/13/2019 The Temporal Pattern of Shame and Guilt

    6/29

    The temporal pattern of Shame and Guilt

    6

    Contents

    LITERATURE REVIEW ........................................................................................................... 7

    SIMULATION HEURISTIC............................................................................................................................................. 7

    REGRET................................................................................................................................................................ 10

    SHAME AND GUILT,WHAT IS THE DIFFERENCE? ............................................................................................................ 13

    HYPOTHESES......................................................................................................................................................... 14

    METHOD ................................................................................................................................. 16

    RESULTS................................................................................................................................. 18

    DISCUSSION .......................................................................................................................... 21

    REFERENCES ......................................................................................................................... 24

    APPENDIX A: ADAPTED VERSION OF THE TEST OF SELF-CONSCIOUS AFFECT..27

  • 8/13/2019 The Temporal Pattern of Shame and Guilt

    7/29

    The temporal pattern of Shame and Guilt

    7

    Literature review

    Simulation heuristic

    Humankind has the ability to do a remarkable thing: it can travel forward or backward in

    subjective time, that is: reviewing and thinking about past events, which can be viewed as

    mental time travelling (Mandel, Hilton & Catellani, 2005). Mandeland colleagues (2005,

    p.1) describe this feature of the human mind as a truly outstanding evolutionary feat, one that

    has propelled our species far beyond even the most formidable powers of retrospection.

    Kahneman and Tversky (1982) were among the first to theorize about this phenomenon.

    They describe two classes of mental operations that can bring things to the mind: retrieval or

    construction. With regard to the construction class they come up with a mental operation they

    term the simulation heuristic. This simulation heuristic holds that people run events through

    in their minds chronologically to assess likely consequences and to aid judgement. Examples

    of judgmental activities in which mental simulation appears to be involved are (Kahneman &

    Tversky, 1982): prediction (for example when imagining how people will behave in the

    future), assessing the probability of a specified event (e.g. how do you assess the likelihood

    of American armed intervention in Saudi Arabia in the next decade?) assessing conditioned

    probabilities (e.g. if civil war breaks out in Saudi Arabia, what are the likely consequences?)

    or assessments of causality (to test whether event A caused event B, we may undo event A in

    our mind, and observe whether B still occurs in the simulation).

    Another important example of the simulation heuristic (on which the focus lies in this

    research paper) iscounterfactual reasoning; the mental simulation of how events might have

    occurred in a different manner(Fiske & Taylor, 2008). Roese and Olson (1995) argue that

    this ability to imagine alternative (i.e. counterfactual) outcomes is an omnipresent and

    essential feature of our (mental) lives.

    Counterfactuals are frequently conditional propositions that include both an antecedent

    and a consequent (e.g. if A (antecedent) than B (consequent); Roese, 1997). Kahneman

    and Tversky (1982) proposed a series of hypotheses. One of these was that counterfactual

    simulations are normality-restoring. For example, in their study, participants read a scenario

    about Mr. Jones, a 47 year old father of three and a successful banking executive. There were

    two versions of the scenario. In the first version Mr. Jones left his office at the normal time

    but he took a different route as than he normally did (route-scenario). In the second version of

    the story, Mr. Jones left the office early to take care of some household chores, but did drive

  • 8/13/2019 The Temporal Pattern of Shame and Guilt

    8/29

    The temporal pattern of Shame and Guilt

    8

    home along his normal route. The scenario also included the information that he occasionally

    took a different route along the shore, but did not do so in this instance. In both versions of the

    scenario Mr. Jones got involved in a car accident in which he is killed. After reading the

    scenario participants were asked to produce, if only... statements.

    It was found that the subjects that read the route-scenario were likely to undo the accident

    by imagining Mr. Jones driving along his normal route (restoring of normality) instead of

    leaving office at a different time (introducing an exception). After reading the time-scenario

    participants were more likely to imagine Mr. Jones leaving office at the usual time (restoring

    of normality) than think of a version in which he drove along the shore (introducing an

    exception). It appears that it is easier for a person that engages in counterfactual thinking to

    imagine that something that rarely happens would not have happened at all (i.e. easier to

    reason that Mr. Jones would have left the office at the usual time or that he would have driven

    home along his normal route). Only very rarely people tend to undo events by making

    mutations that involve mentally deleting normal antecedents or inventing new ones instead of

    deleting abnormal antecedents.

    Another idea by Kahneman and Tversky (1982) was that the ease of undoing has

    implications for the intensity of emotions that people experience and judgements they make.

    For example, a second scenario depicted two persons, Mr. Crane and Mr. Tees, who were

    scheduled to leave the airport on different flights, at the same time (Kahneman & Tversky,

    1982). Both used the same car and were 30 minutes late for their flights, due to traffic

    congestion. Mr Crane is told that his flight left on time, whereas Mr. Tees is told that his flight

    was delayed and has just left, only 5 minutes ago. Participants were asked to indicate who

    they thought would be more upset. Almost every participant (96 %) stated that Mr. Tees

    would be the person most upset about missing his flight. This is remarkable, because strict

    objectively seen, there is not a real difference in outcome between the two, both are in the

    same situation of missing their flights.

    Kahneman and Tversky (1982) argue that it seems that participants engage in a

    simulation exercise, in which they test how close they came to reaching their flight in time.

    Mr. Tees is now thought to be more disappointed because it is easierfor him to imagine that

    he could have still caught his plane if he would have arrived 5 minutes earlier on the airport,

    whereas it is more difficult for Mr. Tees to imagine a situation in which the 30 minutes delay

    could have been avoided.

  • 8/13/2019 The Temporal Pattern of Shame and Guilt

    9/29

    The temporal pattern of Shame and Guilt

    9

    Norm theory

    To shortly summarize the key concepts proposed by Kahneman and Tversky (1982):

    counterfactuals are normality restoring (1), some alternatives are closer to reality than others

    (2) and the effect this has on the ease of undoing is linked with the intensity of emotions

    experienced (3). These propositions are included and elaborated further upon in norm theory

    (Kahneman and Miller, 1986). Norm theory states that the judgemental and affective

    reactions to past events are influenced by a certain standard of comparison; cognitive

    reference points that are used as a norm (Mandel et al., 2005). People who engage in

    counterfactual thought seek to find restoration of that norm-state. In norm theory,

    counterfactual alternatives are thought to be constructed ad hoc (online).

    An assumption that is derived from norm theory is the emotional amplificationhypothesis(Kahneman & Miller, 1986). According to this hypothesis the affective response to

    an event is enhanced if its causes are abnormal. The findings by Kahneman and Tversky

    (1982) that Mr. Tees would be most upset after missing his flight (with only 5 minutes), for

    example, are a clear exhibition of this hypothesis (because Mr. Tees plane was delayed,

    which is a deviation from normality). Another exhibition of norm theory was demonstrated by

    Miller and McFarland (1986). Participants were presented with a scenario in which a person

    fell victim of a shooting incident while walking to a shop (Miller & McFarland, 1986). In oneversion it was a shop he frequently went by, in another version it was a shop he rarely visited.

    The dependent variable was the amount of money participants awarded to the victim as

    compensation. It was found that the participants awarded significantly more money to the

    victim who went to the shop he usually very rarely frequented (e.g. the more abnormal

    version).

    The emotional amplification hypothesis implies that counterfactuals tend to have a

    direction. They can be either positive when imagined alternative circumstances are evaluated

    better than actuality (upward counterfactuals), or negative when alternative circumstances are

    evaluated worse than actuality (downward counterfactuals; Roese, 1997).

    Both types of counterfactual thinking, downward and upward, seem to have a

    functional basis. Downward counterfactual thinking, for example, can be adaptive in helping

    to (re)gain positive affect (Roese, 1994). As for downward counterfactual thinking, feeling

    bad results in if only-thoughts, which leads to the imagination of a better world (Roese,

    1997). In these situations imagining how things could have happened differently facilitates a

  • 8/13/2019 The Temporal Pattern of Shame and Guilt

    10/29

    The temporal pattern of Shame and Guilt

    10

    better understanding of the world. This reconsideration of the past, and creating alternatives to

    what actually has happened, can be useful in helping to adapt, and improve outcomes in the

    future (e.g. Roese, 1994; Roese, 1997). For example, if Tommy failed an exam and realises

    that he would have passed it, had he studied more, than he has identified a causally antecedent

    action that he can use in the future to improve his performance (Roese, 1997). Although

    upward counterfactuals can have both positive (better understanding of the world) and

    negative consequences (feeling bad) that often stand in a state of tension with each other, the

    net result will thus be positive on average (e.g. better future performance; Roese, 1997).

    Regret

    Counterfactual thinking is associated with specific affective experiences. Regret is, together with

    the self-conscious emotions shame and guilt (see the next paragraph)pre-eminently associated

    with the production of counterfactual thoughts. Gilovich and Medvec (1994, p.357) use an aptly

    metaphor: Regrets are like taxes: nearly everyone must suffer them. It seems almost impossible

    for a person to live his life without regretting anything. Regret is typically felt in response to

    decisions that produce unfavorable outcomes, compared to the alternative (Van Dijk &

    Zeelenberg, 2006). It has been found that regret leads to feelings that one should have known

    better, having a sinking feeling, thoughts about the mistake that was made and opportunities that

    were lost, feeling a tendency to kick oneself, and wanting to undo the event and get a second

    chance (Zeelenberg, van Dijk, Manstead & van der Pligt, 1998).

    One important factor determining the amplitude of regret is whether the emotions was

    caused by actions or inactions. Numerous studies (see for an overview e.g. Gilovich & Medvec,

    1995) show that people experience more regret caused by actions than by failures to act (e.g.

    failing to seize the moment). For example Kahneman and Tversky (1982) let participants read

    the following scenario:

    Mr. Paul owns share in company A. During the past year he considered switching to

    stock in company B, but he decided against it. He now finds out he would have been

    better off by $1.200 if he had switched to the stock of company B. Mr. George owned

    shares in company B. During the past year he switched to stock in company A. He now

    finds that he would have been better off by $1.200 if he had kept his stock in company B.

    Who feels greater regret?

  • 8/13/2019 The Temporal Pattern of Shame and Guilt

    11/29

    The temporal pattern of Shame and Guilt

    11

    Participants answered in great majority (92%) that they thought that Mr. George (who loses

    money because he fails to act) would experience more regret.

    Gilovich and Medvec (1994) elaborated on this theory and came up with the notion of a

    temporal pattern of regret, differentiating between short and long term periods of time after an

    event took place. When participants were asked what they regretted most in life, among the most

    common regrets participants reported were: missed educational opportunities, failures to seize

    the moment, not spending enough time with friends and relatives and rushed in too soon

    (Gilovich and Medvec, 1994, Study 2). Only the last regret is caused by an action, all the others

    regrets imply failures to act. In another study (Gilovich & Medvec, 1994, Study 3 and 4)

    participants were presented with a scenario that read as follows:

    Dave and Jim do not know each other, but both are enrolled at the same elite East CoastUniversity. Both are only moderately satisfied where they are and both are considering

    transferring to another prestigious school. Each agonizes over the decision, going back

    and forth between thinking he is going to stay and thinking he will leave. They ultimately

    make different decisions: Dave opts to stay where he is and Jim decides to transfer.

    Suppose their decisions turn out badly for both of them: Dave still doesn't like it where he

    is and wishes he had transferred, and Jim doesn't like his new environment and wishes he

    had stayed.

    After reading the scenario it was asked which of the persons depicted in the story would

    experience more regret, either in the short term or in the long run. The results were that in the

    short term subjects actions (Jims transfer to a different university) produced more regret than

    their inactions (Dave staying at the university), however, in the long run a reverse effect showed.

    A failure to act led to more regret in the long runthan an action.

    Gilovich and Medvec (1994) give several explanations for this effect. They suggest

    that there is a asymmetry in how easy people engage in behavioral adjustment a situation they

    feel bad about. It is easier for people to follows action with another action than it is to follow

    inaction with an action. Additionally people are more inclined to engage in dissonance

    reduction for actions than for inactions, because tend to feel more responsible for their actions

    than inactions (without responsibility there is less dissonance). Both theories (asymmetries in

    ameliorative behavior and differential dissonance reduction) lead to the diminishing of the

    string of regrettable action (Gilovich & Medvec, 1994, p. 362). That is: the higher amount of

    regret experienced after actions in the short run causes psychological (dissonance reduction)

  • 8/13/2019 The Temporal Pattern of Shame and Guilt

    12/29

    The temporal pattern of Shame and Guilt

    12

    and behavioral mechanisms (reparative action) to come in place which diminish regret.

    Contrarily people do use these strategies less when inactions are concerned and end up

    regretting their failures to act more in the long run. A possible explanation is that the reasons

    why one did not act in the past seem to become less pressing and harder to image (i.e. less

    salient), when time passes (Gilovich & Medvec, 1994). Therefore it may be easier to think

    about why one acted, but more difficult why one did not. In the short-term people rely on

    bottom-up processes that elicit specific memories, whether over time, people rely top-down

    processes. Because of this much less accurate and abstract way of processing, it is harder for

    people to remember the reasons why one did not act, leading to greater regret in the long term

    (Gilovich & Medvec, 1994).

    Furthermore, Leach and Plaks (2009) found that the way in which actions and

    inactions are usually represented plays a major role in assessing the perceived amount of regret

    of events. That is: actions tend to be more concrete than inactions. For a better understanding the

    difference between abstract versus concrete representation it can be useful to image a person

    ringing a doorbell (Leach & Plaks, 2009). This persons behavior can be described either in

    abstract terms as calling on a friend (which is a higher level representation) or concretely as

    pushing a button (lower level representation). A change in a high level representation of an

    event produces major changes in the meaning of the event, whereas a change in a lower level

    representation produces only relatively minor changes in the meaning of the event, which makes

    this a key difference between the two (Trope & Liberman, 2003).

    Construal level theory (Trope & Liberman, 2003) states that details about distant future

    events are often knowable only as the time draws closer, which causes people to associate distant

    future events with abstract concepts (e.g. composing of general, superordinate and essential

    features). When - in contrast - the event is coming nearer, concrete considerations (e.g.,

    subordinate goals, specifics of the situation) are more fruitful for a successful execution of the

    task at hand. Actions tend to be more concrete in that they are more causal than inactions

    (Leach & Plaks, 2009). That is: a strong and concrete subjective link exists between action and

    its effects. Contrarily, such a link is absent for inactions, which leads to a more abstract

    processing. It was found, for example, that participants greater regret for inactions in the distant

    term was mediated by the level of abstraction (Leach & Plaks, 2009); distant term inactions were

    regretted more than actions onlywhen they were represented abstractly. This in turn causes a

    greater memorability for the event and an increased focus on the broader implications.

  • 8/13/2019 The Temporal Pattern of Shame and Guilt

    13/29

    The temporal pattern of Shame and Guilt

    13

    Shame and guilt, what is the difference?

    Both shame and guilt are negatively valenced, self-relevant emotions (Niedental, Tagney &

    Gavanski, 1994). Studies suggest that the function of these emotions is to guide behavior that

    conforms to social and moral norms and to promote restitution from misdeeds (Parrott, 2001).

    Although most people use the terms shame and guilt in an interchangeable manner, there

    have been made numerous attempts to differentiate between the two emotions (see for an

    overview Tangney & Tracy, 2012).

    The main approach in the field has for long been based on the assumption that shame and

    guilt can be differentiated by the types of situations that induce these emotions. Shame was

    thought to be an affective reaction following public exposure (and disapproval) of some

    shortcoming. Guilt on the other hand was thought to be a reaction of the internalized

    conscience of a person when ones personal standards are violated andcould in this manner be

    seen a more private emotion. Evidence for this theory is mixed. For example, Walbott and

    Scherer (1995) conducted a large-scale cross-cultural study in which participants from different

    cultures were asked to recall situations in which they had experienced a whole range of different

    emotions including shame and guilt. It was found that shame experiences were elicited

    significantly more often by other people or by external sources, whereas guilt experiences were

    more often attributed to the self. Walbott and Scherer (1995, p. 174) conclude: This [finding] is

    consistent with the idea that guilt is caused by internal sanctions, whereas shame is caused by

    external sanctions emanating from other people of institutions. On the other hand, there is

    empirical evidence that although shame and guilt occurred most often in social contexts, also

    solitary shame experiences were not uncommon(e.g. Tangney, Miller, Flicker & Barlow,

    1996).

    Helen Block Lewis (1971) was the first to come up with a different theory that draws

    upon the role of the self instead of the role of private-public situations. She theorises that a

    distinction of shame and guilt can be made on the basis of the functional role of the self. In her

    theory the experience of shame is associated with the self, which is the focus of evaluation,

    whereas the self is not the central object of evaluation in guilt. Instead, the focus in guilt lies on

    the thingdone or undone. As a result shame leads to a fairly global self-condemnation, whereas

    guilt is experienced somewhat apart from the self (Niedenthal et al., 1994).

    A consequence of this is that shame and guilt seem to be involved in different types of

    counterfactual thinking. For example, Niedenthal and colleagues (1994, study 1) asked subjects

  • 8/13/2019 The Temporal Pattern of Shame and Guilt

    14/29

    The temporal pattern of Shame and Guilt

    14

    to read about two situations inducing shame or guilt and then to undo these situations (i.e.

    generating counterfactuals).

    It was found that subjects tended to undo the shame inducing situations by altering the

    self, whereas subjects tended to undo guilt situations by altering actions (behavior). In a second

    study (Niedental et al., 1994, study 2), participants read about an ambiguous event that could

    induce both shame and guilt. Subsequently, participants were asked to produce counterfactuals in

    which they either altered the self or behavior. This led to similar results. Subjects who were

    asked to change the self, reported to experience more shame subsequently, whereas subjects who

    were asked to change behavior experienced more guilt. Tracy and Robins (2006) found that

    individuals do not regulate feelings of guilt through making external attributions. Instead, they

    make internal attributions, which lead to an even greater experience of guilt. It seems that guilt is

    regulated behavioral rather than cognitive (Tracy & Robins, 2006). Guilt motivates reparative

    behavior (apology, future hard work). In contrast shame leads to blaming stable, uncontrollable

    aspects of the self (Tracy & Robins, 2006). Therefore, an ashamed person has to adopt a long-

    term strategy of behavioral modification to reduce the emotional experience of shame. That is:

    working toward becoming a different kind of person.

    Hypotheses

    In the present study the main aim is to investigate whether actions and failures to act have also

    consequences for the experience of shame and guilt. In addition it is investigated whether the

    temporal pattern reported by Gilovich and Medvec (1994) exists for the experience of shame and

    guilt as well. In the literature it has been shown that some aspects of reality are appear more

    mutable than others, depending on the relative ease to which aspects of reality can be altered to

    produce counterfactuals (Kahneman & Miller, 1986).

    This concept could be useful to make a distinction between shame and guilt. Shame is

    pre-eminently an emotion focused on the self, whereas guilt is focused on situational factors

    (e.g. Niedenthal et al., 1994). Because the self-concept is viewed as relatively stable over time

    (e.g. Demo, 1992) it is hypothesized that events inducing shame are less mutable than events

    inducing guilt. This idea hinges on the premise that it is considerably easier for a person to

    imagine how a situationcould have been different (if only I didt), compared to how he himself

    could be different (if only I werent). As a result it is assumed that the reversed effect on the

    time dimension (inactions induce more regret in the long run) will show for situations inducing

  • 8/13/2019 The Temporal Pattern of Shame and Guilt

    15/29

    The temporal pattern of Shame and Guilt

    15

    guilt, but not shame.

    It short is expected that:

    - In the short-term subjects experience greater guilt stemming from actions than from

    inactions (hypothesis 1);

    - In the long run subjects experience greater guilt stemming from inactions than from

    actions (hypothesis 2);

    - No such time effect exists for the experience of shame (hypothesis 3). That is: in both

    the short-term and long run subjects experience greater shame from inactions than from

    actions.

  • 8/13/2019 The Temporal Pattern of Shame and Guilt

    16/29

    The temporal pattern of Shame and Guilt

    16

    Method

    Participants and design.234 undergraduate Psychology students at the University ofGroningen took part in the present study, of which 70 were male and 164 female. The

    youngest participant was 18 years old, the oldest 29, whereas the mean age of all the

    participants was 20.35. The participants received credits in exchange for their participation

    and were randomly assigned to one of the six experimental conditions. The study can be

    characterized as a 2 (shame vs. guilt) x 2 (short vs. long term) x 2 (action vs. inaction)

    between subjects design.

    Procedure. After completing an assent form, participants read one of six scenarios,

    which were an adaption of the original scenarios used byNiedenthal, Tangley and Gavanski(1994). The participants were asked to carefully read the scenario and to imagine experiencing

    the situation that was described. It was noted that it was not important for the experiment

    whether they thought the experiences could actually happen, but that the researchers were

    interested in their thoughts about the experiences if they were to happen. The scenario

    inducing shame stemming from an action (scenario 1a) read as follows (with the short vs.

    long term conditions in bold):

    Imagine yourself sitting in a class of 15 students that is taught by a professor you admire

    more than any other on campus. You really believe that he/she is brilliant. The professor

    asks a question about one of the readings/assignments and you immediately volunteer

    the answer with some enthusiasm. But its the wrong answer, the professor informs you

    somewhat dryly and he/she turns to address another student in the class.

    Please image that this event [has happened 1 day ago / has happened six months ago].

    The scenario inducing shame stemming from an inaction (scenario 1b) read as follows:

    Imagine yourself sitting in a class of 15 students that is taught by a professor you admire

    more than any other on campus. You really believe that he/she is brilliant. The professor

    asks a question about one of the readings/assignments where you have previously

    written an essay about. Everybody in class expects you to answer it. But you seem

    paralyzed and keep quiet. The professor looks disappointed and addresses another

  • 8/13/2019 The Temporal Pattern of Shame and Guilt

    17/29

    The temporal pattern of Shame and Guilt

    17

    student in the class.

    Please image that this event [has happened 1 day ago / has happened six months ago].

    The scenario inducing guilt stemming from an action (scenario 2a) read as follows:

    You are house-sitting for a friends parents. Its a pretty easy task actually. All you have

    to do is eat their food, collect the mail, and feed their bird. Everything is going just fine

    until one morning you discover that the bird died during the night. You did not feed him

    with the bird food as you were instructed but you bought some nuts on the market

    yourself. The bird died from choking while swallowing a particularly large nut.

    Please image that this event [has happened 1 day ago / has happened six months ago].

    The scenario inducing guilt stemming from an inaction (scenario 2b) read as follows:

    You are house-sitting for a friends parents. Its a pretty easy task actually. All you have

    to do is eat their food, collect the mail, and feed their bird. Everything is going just fine

    until one morning you discover that the bird died during the night. You forgot to turn the

    air conditioning down at night as you had been instructed. The bird died from the

    excessive cold.

    Please image that this event [has happened 1 day ago / has happened six months ago].

    After this, participant were asked to indicate to what extent they agreed with a number of

    statements adapted from the Test of Self-Conscious Affect (TOSCA; Tangney, Wagner &

    Gramzow, 1989). The ratings were done on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from totally agree till

    totally disagree.

    An example of a statement measuring shame is You would feel like you wanted to hide. An

    example of a statement indicating feelings of guilt is You would feel unhappy and eager to

    correct the situation(the complete survey is added as a supplement to this thesis).

    Manipulation checks. After these statements participants answered three questions that

    served as a manipulation check. They were asked how long ago the events portrayed in the

    scenario were supposed to happen (time manipulation check) and whether he or she thought

    the event happened because he did something or did not do something (action vs. inaction

    manipulation check). After this, the participant indicated whether other people knew about the

  • 8/13/2019 The Temporal Pattern of Shame and Guilt

    18/29

    The temporal pattern of Shame and Guilt

    18

    events that had happened (guilt-shame manipulation check).Finally the participant was asked

    how big the change would be that he would feel the same way in a similar situation.

    Results

    The data met the assumptions of homogeneity of variance. The dependent variable shame was

    normally distributed (for the groups that consisted of the combination of the levels of action

    (action versus failure to act) and time (long versus short; Shapiro-Wilk > .05). There was an

    indication that the dependent variable guilt was not normally distributed (Shapiro-Wilk

  • 8/13/2019 The Temporal Pattern of Shame and Guilt

    19/29

    The temporal pattern of Shame and Guilt

    19

    Guilt scenario measuring guilt

    Similarly, a two-way analysis of variance with time and event-type as the independent

    variables and guilt as the independent variable showed a main effect on the time dimension

    (F(1,100) = 10.71,p= .01), for the subjects that were presented with the guilt scenario.

    Again, participants reported experiencing less guilt if they read about events that happened a

    long time ago (M= 3.22, SD= .67), versus not long ago (M= 3.63, SD= .57).

    Shame scenario measuring shame

    A significant effect existed between the scenario in which an action was described compared

    to the scenario in which an inaction was described (F(1,126) = 10.538 ,p= .01). It was found

    that when subjects read about an event that induced shame because they failed to do

    something, they reported more shame (M= 2.93, SD= .73), than when they read a scenario in

    which they did something (M= 2.49, SD= .73). No other effects were significant.

    Shame scenario measuring guilt

    Interestingly, also a significant difference between actions (M= 2.73, SD= .76) and inactions

    (M= 3.21, SD= .67) was found for guilt when participants were presented with the shame

    scenario (F(1,126) = 14.45,p< .01), that is: participants reported more guilt over failures to

    act. No other significant effects were found.

    Repeated measures ANOVA for the guilt scenarios

    A repeated measures ANOVA with a Greenhouse-Geisser correction determined that mean

    ratings on the guilt scale differed significantly from mean ratings on the shame scale for the

    participants that were presented with a guilt scenario (F(1, 100) = 56.80,p< 0.01). In

    accordance with what one might expect, indeed ratings on the guilt scale (M= 3.41, SD= .66)

    were higher than on the shame scale (M= 2.97, SD= .74). Additionally it was found that a

    significant effect existed with respect to the time dimension (F(1,100) = 12.16,p< .01).

    Repeated measures ANOVA for the shame scenarios

    Similarly, a repeated measures ANOVA with a Greenhouse-Geisser correction showed that

    mean ratings on the guilt scale differed statistically significantly from mean ratings on the

    shame scale for the participants that were presented with a shame scenario (F(1, 126) = 26.17,

    p< .01). Contrary to what one would expect, ratings on the guilt scale were higher (M = 2.75,

    SD = .76), than on the shame scale (M= 2.73, SD= .76), indicating that participants did

  • 8/13/2019 The Temporal Pattern of Shame and Guilt

    20/29

    The temporal pattern of Shame and Guilt

    20

    experience more guilt instead of shame when confronted with a shame scenario. An

    significant effect existed between the scenario in which an action was described compared to

    the one in which an failure to act was described (F(1,126) = 14.75,p< 0.01), indicating that

    ratings on the shame and guilt scales were on average higher for inactions than actions.

    Manipulation checks

    Almost all participants answered the time manipulation check (How long ago did the events

    portrayed in the story happen?) correctly. Two participants answered that they did not now

    and one answered 34 (without specification whether this were days or months). Another two

    participants answered that they read about events that happened 2 years ago, whereas another

    answered 4 weeks (this actually was the 6 months condition), and one participant reported

    that the events were supposed to happen 3 years ago (whereas he or she was in the 1 day

    condition). With regard to whether the events in the scenario happened because of an action

    or an inaction 33 participants (of 234 in total) failed this check, whereas 41 failed the guilt-

    shame manipulation check. The participants indicated a change of average 70.14 %

    (SD=22.55) that they would feel the same in a similar situation.

    An additional analysis of the data was carried out, in which all participants that failed

    one or more manipulation checks were eliminated. In total 79 responses were eliminated. All

    effects of the previous analysis were found. Only, the finding that the participants that read

    the guilt scenario reported less shame in the long run was not replicated, that is: in the second

    analysis no effect was found.

  • 8/13/2019 The Temporal Pattern of Shame and Guilt

    21/29

    The temporal pattern of Shame and Guilt

    21

    Discussion

    Previous research on counterfactual thinking has shown that people tend to experience more

    regret over actions than inactions on the short run, but as time passes, a shift takes place: people

    tend to experience more regret because of things they failed to do compared to the things they

    did (Gilovich & Medvec, 1994). A possible explanation is that the reasons why one did not act in

    the past seem to become less pressing and harder to image (i.e. less salient), when time passes

    (Gilovich & Medvec, 1994). Therefore it may be easier to think about why one acted, but more

    difficult why one did not. Peoples regret may thus intensify because the failure to act in the past

    seems inexplicable to them in the present.

    In the present research it was expected that the time effect observed for the experience of

    regret would exist for the experience of shame and guilt as well. Since shame is pre-eminently an

    emotion focused on the self (whereas guilt is focused on situational factors) and the self-

    concept is viewed as relatively stable over time it was hypothesized that events inducing shame

    are less mutable than events inducing guilt. This idea hinges on the premise that it is

    considerably easier for a person to imagine how a situation could have been different (if only I

    didt), compared to how his own behavior could be different (if only I werent). It was

    expected that in the short-term subjects experience greater guilt stemming from actions than

    from inactions (hypothesis 1). Furthermore, it was expected that in the long run subjects would

    experience greater guilt stemming from inactions than from actions (hypothesis 2) and that no

    such time effect exists for the experience of shame (hypothesis 3), that is: subjects experience

    more guilt over inactions in both the short term and the long run.

    The results did not give support for this hypothesis. When participants were presented with

    the bird scenario (inducing guilt) they experienced overall more guilt in the short term (compared

    to the long run), but this was independent of event type (action and inaction). When participants

    were presented with the professor scenario (inducing shame) a significant difference between

    event type (action and inaction) was found. That is: overall, participants who read the professor

    scenario experienced more shame as a consequence of something they failed to do compared to

    something they did. This finding is in line with the idea that simulations are more likely to

    increase the perceived likelihood of a potential outcome than to reduce the probability or extent

    of an outcome. It has been shown in previous research (e.g. Dunning and Parpal, 1989), that in

    general simulations are more likely to increase the perceived likelihood of a potential outcome

  • 8/13/2019 The Temporal Pattern of Shame and Guilt

    22/29

    The temporal pattern of Shame and Guilt

    22

    (mental addition) than to reduce the probability or extent of an outcome (mental subtraction).

    Dunning and Parpal (1989, study 1) asked subjects to indicate how many morequestions on a

    test they would get right if they studied 3 ours longer. Alternatively they asked how manyfewer

    questions subjects would get right if they studied three hours less. Subjects estimated a greater

    impact when presented with the additive frame (studying 3 ours longer), than when presented

    with the subtractive frame (studying three hours less). In other words: although the time period

    of studying or not-studying was the same, subjects thought that studying for three hours more

    would result in a greater change in the amount of right answers, than studying less would. In a

    second study Dunning and Parpal (1989, study 2) asked students at Stanford University in what

    extent coming to Stanford as opposed to the university of their second choice had influenced

    their education, social life and future prospects. Other students estimated the potential

    consequences of attending their second favorite university. Also in this study subjects perceived

    greater impact when they were confronted with an additive (Stanford) as opposed to a subtractive

    frame (university of second choice). The effect seems to exist because people give more weight

    to features of the particular mental stimulation serving as the subject of comparison and place a

    higher value in factors that produce instead of inhibit the relevant outcome (Dunning & Parpal,

    1989). People that engage in counterfactual thought because they have done something are

    eliminating that outcome (i.e. subtraction; if only I hadnt done that). On the other hand, people

    that engage in counterfactual thought because they did not do something are adding an outcome

    (i.e. mental addition; if only I did that). When people are thinking back about an event in

    which they failed to do something (inaction), compared to when they did do something (action),

    imagining that they wouldhave acted will have greater impact.

    In the present study (counterfactual) thinking about not having answered the question of the

    professor (if only I didnt, which is mental subtraction) is harder for people than thinking about

    having answered the question (if only I did, which is mental addition). Because it is easier for

    participants to come up with counterfactuals for the outcome caused by an inaction (death of the

    bird by leaving a window open), they experience a higher amount of shame.

    However, a difference in time between actions and inactions (short versus long ago) such as

    in the classic regret studies by Gilovich and Medvec (1994) was not found. Morrison and Roese

    (2011, p.580) name an important disadvantage of using students as participants in research:

    College student samples are quick and cheap, but whether effects gleaned from such samples

    generalize to the wider population remains a key challenge for psychological theory.In their

    study they investigated real-life regrets using a national representative sample in the US and they

  • 8/13/2019 The Temporal Pattern of Shame and Guilt

    23/29

    The temporal pattern of Shame and Guilt

    23

    did replicate the time effect (regrets over inactions were more common over long term). The

    notion that using students as participants can be problematic applies to the present study as well:

    a percentage between 10-20 % of the participants failed multiple manipulation checks. Another

    important shortcoming of the present study is the use of a scenario design, which leads to a

    diminishing of experimental realism compared to surveys assessing real life emotions (such as

    the study by Morrison and Roese, 2011). On the other hand: the participants did indicate

    indicated a chance of on average 70.14 % that they would feel the same in a similar (real world)

    situation. The clear disadvantage of asking participants about actual experiences is that it is

    harder to compare their answers with each other (as opposed to using a scenario). However,

    Morrison and Roese (2011) give a clear example of the fact that it is possible, however time

    consuming and costly.

    A rather curious finding was that participants reported experiencing a higher amount of guilt

    than shame when they were presented with a shame scenario (about the professor). Apparently

    the scenario did produce more guilt than shame. Niedenthal and colleagues (1994), however, did

    find in a pilot study, using the exact same professor scenario, that participants rated the scenario

    4.82 on the shame scale and 2.51 on the guilt scale. In the present study the participants rated the

    professor scenario 2.75 on the guilt scale and 2.73 on the shame scale. Part of the difference can

    be explained by measurement differences: the scale Niedenthal and colleagues used was a 7

    point scale, whereas in the present study a 5 point scale was used. However, why participants

    rated a higher amount of guilt instead of shame for the professor scenario (that was supposed to

    induce shame) cannot be explained by this.

    Conclusion

    A temporal effect for actions and inactions in the experience of shame and guilt, such as Gilovic

    and Medvec (1994) found (in which actions produce more regret in the short term and inactions

    in the long run) failed to show in this study. However, it was found that participants that read a

    scenario inducing shame experienced more shame as a consequence of something they failed to

    do compared to something they did (without a time effect). Participants that read a scenario

    inducing guilt experienced more guilt in the short term, compared to the long run. To further

    investigate how these emotions operate in time, additional research is needed, in whichas a

    recommendationreal life emotional events are taken into account.

  • 8/13/2019 The Temporal Pattern of Shame and Guilt

    24/29

    The temporal pattern of Shame and Guilt

    24

    References

    Demo, D. (1992). The self-concept over time: Research issues and directions.Annual Review

    of Sociology, 18(1992), 303326

    Dunning, D., & Parpal, M. (1989). Mental addition versus subtraction in counterfactual

    reasoning: on assessing the impact of personal actions and life events. Journal of

    Personality and Social Psychology, 57(1), 515.

    Fiske, S., & Taylor, S. (2008). Social cognition. From brains to culture. New York, NY:

    McGraw-Hill.

    Gilovich, T., & Medvec, V. H. (1994). The temporal pattern to the experience of regret.

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(3), 35765. Retrieved from

    http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/7965599

    Gilovich, T., & Medvec, V. H. (1995). The experience of regret: what, when, and why.

    Psychological Review, 102(2), 37995. Retrieved from

    http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22115451

    Kahneman, D., & Miller, D. (1986). Norm theory: Comparing reality to its alternatives.

    Psychological review. Retrieved from http://psycnet.apa.org/journals/rev/93/2/136/

    Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1982). The simulation heuristic. In D. Kahneman, P. Slovic, &

    A. Tversky,Judgement Under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases(pp. 201-208). New

    York: Cambridge university press.

    Leach, F. R., & Plaks, J. E. (2009). Regret for errors of commission and omission in the

    distant term versus near term: the role of level of abstraction.Personality & social

    psychology bulletin, 35(2), 2219. doi:10.1177/0146167208327001

    Lewis, H. B. (1971). Shame and guilt in neurosis. New York: International Universities Press.

    Mandel, David R. Hilton, D. J., & Catellani, P. C. (Eds.). (2005). The Psychology ofCounterfactual Thinking. London: Routledge.

  • 8/13/2019 The Temporal Pattern of Shame and Guilt

    25/29

    The temporal pattern of Shame and Guilt

    25

    Morrison, M., & Roese, N. J. (2011). Regrets of the Typical American: Findings From aNationally Representative Sample. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 2(6),576583. doi:10.1177/1948550611401756

    Miller, D. T., & McFarland, C. (1986). Counterfactual Thinking and Victim Compensation: A

    Test of Norm Theory.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 12(4), 513519.

    doi:10.1177/0146167286124014

    Miller, Dale T., & Gunasegaram, S. (1990). Temporal order and the perceived mutability of

    events: Implications for blame assignment.Journal of Personality and Social

    Psychology, 59(6), 11111118. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.59.6.1111

    Niedenthal, P. M., Tangney, J. P., & Gavanski, I. (1994). If only I werent versus if only I

    hadnt: distinguishing shame and guilt in counterfactual thinking.Journal of

    Personality and Social Psychology, 67(4), 58595.

    Parrott, W. (2001). The nature of emotion. In T. A., & N. Schwarz,Blackwell Handbook of

    Social Psychology: Intraindividual Processes(pp. 376-390). Oxford: Blackwell

    Publishers.

    Roese, N. J., & Olson, J. M. (1995). What Might Have Been: The Social Psychology ofCounterfactual Thinking. New Jersey: Erlbaum.

    Roese, N J. (1997). Counterfactual thinking.Psychological bulletin, 121(1), 13348.

    Roese, Neal J. (1994). The functional basis of counterfactual thinking. Journal of Personality

    and Social Psychology, 66(5), 805818. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.66.5.805

    Tangney, J. (1996). Conceptual and methodological issues in the assessment of shame and

    guilt.Behaviour research and therapy, 34(9), 741754.

    Tangney, J., & Dearing, R. (2003). Shame and guilt(pp. 447450) New York: Guilford.

    Tangney, J. P., Miller, R. S., Flicker, L., & Barlow, D. H. (1996). Are shame, guilt, and

    embarrassment distinct emotions?Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70(6),

    125669.

  • 8/13/2019 The Temporal Pattern of Shame and Guilt

    26/29

    The temporal pattern of Shame and Guilt

    26

    Tangney, J., & Tracy, J. L. (2012). Self-conscious emotions. In M. Leary & J. Tangney(Eds.),Handbook of self and identity(pp. 446478).

    Tangney, J.P., Wagner, P., & Gramzow, R. (1989). The Test of Self-Conscious Affect.

    (TOSCA). Fairfax: George Mason University.

    Tracy, J. L., & Robins, R. W. (2006). Appraisal antecedents of shame and guilt: support for a

    theoretical model.Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 32(10), 133951.

    doi:10.1177/0146167206290212

    Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2003). Temporal construal.Psychological Review, 110(3), 403

    421. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.110.3.403

    Van Dijk, E., & Zeelenberg, M. (2006). The dampening effect of uncertainty on positive andnegative emotions.Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 19(2), 171176.doi:10.1002/bdm.504

    Wallbott, H., & Scherer, K. (1995). Cultural determinants in experiencing shame and guilt. In:J.P. Tangney & K.W. Fischer (Eds.), Self-conscious emotions.New York: Guilford

    Zeelenberg, Marcel, van Dijk, W. W., Manstead, A., & der Pligt, J. (1998). The Experience of

    Regret and Disappointment. Cognition & Emotion, 12(2), 221230.

    doi:10.1080/026999398379727

  • 8/13/2019 The Temporal Pattern of Shame and Guilt

    27/29

    The temporal pattern of Shame and Guilt

    27

    Appendix A: adapted version of the Test of Self-Conscious Affect (TOSCA; Tangney, Wagner

    & Gramzow, 1989).

    Totally

    disagreeDisagree Neutral Agree

    Totally

    agree

    You would feel like

    you wanted to hidex x x x x

    You would feel

    inadequatex x x x x

    You would feel stupid x x x x x

    You would feel

    incompetentx x x x x

    You would feel

    disgusted with your

    lack of will power

    and self-control

    x x x x x

    You would feel

    immaturex x x x x

    You would feel like a

    cowardx x x x x

    You would think you

    are basically lazy

    x x x x x

    You would think:

    This is making me

    anxious. I need either

    fix it or get someone

    else to

    x x x x x

  • 8/13/2019 The Temporal Pattern of Shame and Guilt

    28/29

    The temporal pattern of Shame and Guilt

    28

    Totally

    disagreeDisagree Neutral Agree

    Totally

    agree

    You would think:

    Im terriblex x x x x

    You would feel small

    ... like a mousex x x x x

    You would probably

    avoid eye-contact for

    a long time

    x x x x x

    You would feel alone

    and apart from other

    people

    x x x x x

    You would think

    about quittingx x x x x

    You would feel: I

    deserve to be

    reprimanded

    x x x x x

    You would keep quiet

    and avoid other

    people

    x x x x x

    You would feel

    unhappy and eager to

    correct the situation

    x x x x x

    You would feel you

    should not accept itx x x x x

  • 8/13/2019 The Temporal Pattern of Shame and Guilt

    29/29